翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Process.h
・ Processability theory
・ Processed Beats
・ Processed Book Project
・ Processed cheese
・ Processed meat
・ Processed Vegetable Growers' Association
・ Processed World (magazine)
・ Processidae
・ Processing (Chinese materia medica)
・ Processing (programming language)
・ Processing amplifier
・ Processing and Control News Europe
・ Processing delay
・ Processing fluency
Processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure
・ Processing Instruction
・ Processing Magazine
・ Processing medium
・ Processing modes
・ Processing.js
・ Procession
・ Procession (album)
・ Procession (band)
・ Procession (disambiguation)
・ Procession (New Order song)
・ Procession (sculpture)
・ Procession (The Moody Blues song)
・ Procession in St. Mark's Square
・ Procession Kiosk


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure : ウィキペディア英語版
Processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure
The processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure〔Reber, R., Schwarz, N., Winkielman, P.: "Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver's processing experience?", ''Personality and Social Psychology Review'', 8(4):364-382.〕 is a theory in psychological aesthetics on how people experience beauty. Processing fluency is the ease with which information is processed in the human mind. The theory is based on four basic assumptions:
#Objects differ in the fluency with which they can be processed. Variables that facilitate fluent processing include objective features of stimuli, like goodness of form, symmetry, figure-ground contrast, as well as experience with a stimulus, for example repeated exposure or prototypicality.
#Processing fluency is itself hedonically marked (that is, it possesses an inherent affective quality) and high fluency is subjectively experienced as positive.〔Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T., & Reber, R. (2003). The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), ''The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion''. (pp. 189-217). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.〕
#In line with the "feelings-as-information" account,〔Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), ''Social Psychology: Handbook of basic principles'' (2nd ed., pp. 385-407). New York: Guilford.〕 processing fluency feeds into judgments of aesthetic appreciation because people draw on their subjective experience in making evaluative judgments, unless the informational value of the experience is called into question.
#The impact of fluency is moderated by expectations and attribution. On one hand, fluency has a particularly strong impact on affective experience if its source is unknown and fluent processing comes as a surprise. On the other hand, the fluency-based affective experience is discounted as a source of relevant information when the perceiver attributes the experience to an irrelevant source. This helps explain the inverted U-shaped function often found in research on the effect of complexity on preferences:〔Berlyne, D. E. (1971). ''Aesthetics and psychobiology''. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.〕 very complex patterns are not judged as beautiful because they are disfluent, and patterns are judged as more beautiful when they become less complex. When viewers perceive a simple pattern, they are often able to detect the source of fluency—the pattern's simplicity—and do not use this experience of ease for judging the beauty of the pattern.
The processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure emphasizes the interaction between the viewer and an object in that it integrates theories and a wide range of empirical evidence that focus on effects of objective stimulus attributes on perceived beauty〔Birkhoff, G. D. (1933). ''Aesthetic measure''. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.〕 with those that emphasize the role of experience, for example by invoking prototypicality.〔Martindale, C., & Moore, K. (1988). Priming, prototypicality, and preference. ''Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance'', ''14'', 661–670.〕 In this theory, beauty is seen as an experience that has nothing to do with artistic merit: Beautiful works of art may be without any merit whereas good art is not necessarily beautiful.
The theory resolves the apparent paradox of inborn and acquired preferences. For instance, infants prefer consonant melodies. According to the fluency account, this is because infants share perceptual equipment that make them process consonance in music more easily than dissonance. When children grow up, they are exposed to the music of their culture, resulting in culture-specific musical fluency. This familiarization explains why individuals from different cultures have different musical tastes. In addition, the theory helps explain why beauty (in a wide sense; perhaps the term ''elegance'' is more apt) is a cue for truth in mathematical problem solving and scientific discovery.〔Schwarz, N. (2006). On judgments of truth & beauty. ''Daedalus'', ''135'', 136–138.〕〔Reber, R. Brun, M., & Mitterndorfer, K. (2008). The use of heuristics in intuitive mathematical judgment. ''Psychonomic Bulletin & Review'', ''15'', 1174-1178.〕
The theory and its implications have influenced theory and research in the psychology of perception,〔Topolinski , S. (2010). Moving the eye of the beholder: Motor components in vision determine aesthetic preference. ''Psychological Science'', ''21'', 1220-1224.〕 cognitive psychology,〔Opacic, T., Stevens, C., & Tillmann, B. (2009). Unspoken knowledge: Implicit learning of structured human dance movement. ''Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition'', ''35'', 1570–1577.〕 social psychology,〔Rubin, M., Paolinia, S., & Crisp, R. J. (2010). A processing fluency explanation of bias against migrants. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 21–28.〕 empirical aesthetics,〔Plumhoff, J. E. & Schirillo, J. A. (2009). Mondrian, Eye Movements, and the Oblique Effect. ''Perception'', ''38'', 719–731.〕 web design,〔Thielsch, M. T., & Hirschfeld, G. (2010). High and low spatial frequencies in website evaluations. ''Ergonomics'', ''53'', 972-978.〕 〔Thielsch, M. T., & Hirschfeld, G. (2012). Spatial frequencies in aesthetic website evaluations – explaining how ultra-rapid evaluations are formed. ''Ergonomics'', ''55'', 731-742.〕 marketing,〔Lee, A. Y., & Labroo, A. A. (2004). The Effect of Conceptual and Perceptual Fluency on Brand Evaluation. ''Journal of Marketing Research'', ''41'' (2004): 151–165.〕〔Labroo, A. A., Dhar, R., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Of frogs, wines, and frowning watches: Semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. ''Journal of Consumer Research'', ''34'', 819-831.〕 finance, and archeology.〔Hodgson, D. (2009). Symmetry and humans: reply to Mithen's 'Sexy Handaxe Theory'. ''Antiquity'', ''83'', 195-198.〕
== References ==


抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Processing fluency theory of aesthetic pleasure」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.